georgecmu 15 hours ago

For whatever reason, evolution decided those wavelengths should be overlapping. For example, M cones are most sensitive to 535 nm light, while L cones are most sensitive to 560 nm light. But M cones are still stimulated quite a lot by 560 nm light—around 80% of maximum.

The reason is simple: genes coding the long wave opsins (light-sensitive proteins) in these cones have diverged from copies of the same original gene. The evolution of this is very interesting.

Mammals in general have only two types of cones: presumably they lost full color vision in the age of dinosaurs since they were primarily small nocturnal animals or lived in habitats with very limited light (subterranean, piles of leaves, etc.) Primates are the notable exception, and have evolved the third type of cone, enabling trichromatic color vision, as a result of their fruitarian specialization and co-evolution with the tropical fruit trees (same as birds, actually).

So, what's interesting is that New World and Old World primates evolved this cone independently. In Old World primates the third cone resulted from a gene duplication event on the X chromosome, giving rise to two distinct (but pretty similar) opsin genes, with sensitivity peaks at very close wavelengths. As a note, because these genes sit on the X chromosome, colorblindness (defects in one or both of these genes) is much more likely to happen in males.

New World primates have a single polymorphic opsin gene on the X chromosome, with different alleles coding for different sensitivities. So, only some (heterozygous) females in these species typically have full trichromatic vision, while males and the unlucky homozygous females remain dichromatic.

Decent wikipedia article on the subject: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Evolution_of_color_vision_in_p...

Types of opsins in vertebrates: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Vertebrate_visual_opsin

  • cyborgx7 18 minutes ago

    I'm pretty sure that line of the article didn't mean to imply that we don't know, or aren't sure, only that it goes beyond the scope of the article and isn't directly relevant to the topic at hand.

  • __MatrixMan__ 13 hours ago

    This is only tangentially related, but I have always wondered why chlorophyll absorbs blue and red, but reflects green--green being sunlight's brightest component.

    It's almost as if there was some evolutionary pressure towards being very visible in sunlight which is more important than evolving ways to collect as much sun energy as possible. When I guess at this I end up with something along the lines of reflected green being used as a signal to a neighboring plant: "I'm already here, grow in some other direction instead." There is some evidence that plants do this (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Crown_shyness, https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1365-3040.ep1160...) but it's not clear that the need to do so is so strong that it would overshadow the drive to collect as much energy as possible.

    Or perhaps there's something to do with the physics of absorbing light to drive a chemical reaction that makes it better to absorb at red and blue while passing on green (450nm and 680nm are not harmonics--so if this is the case it's more complex than which sorts of standing waves would fit in some chemical gap or other).

    • adrian_b an hour ago

      Chlorophyll a, which is the pigment that actually uses solar energy to split water, absorbs red light and violet light. Thus its color is blue-green, as it can be seen in some lichens that have only symbiotic cyanobacteria.

      This is most likely a historical accident, with no special meaning.

      Most algae and plants have auxiliary pigments, which absorb other parts of the solar spectrum and then transfer the energy to chlorophyll a.

      The land plants and the green algae use mostly chlorophyll b as auxiliary pigment, which absorbs light in a blue band adjacent to the violet band of chlorophyll a, and in a red band that is distinct and adjacent to the red band of chlorophyll a.

      Thus the addition of chlorophyll b increases considerably the amount of captured energy.

      The algae that are dominant in oceans, e.g. diatoms and brown algae, have more auxiliary pigments, so that many are dark brown, even close to black.

      Unlike for marine algae, for land plants, capturing more solar energy is not desirable, because they already have difficulties in avoiding overheating and excessive loss of water. So the pigments used by them are good enough for their needs.

    • cornstalks 10 hours ago

      > green being sunlight's brightest component.

      It actually peaks between magenta and blue: https://sunwindsolar.com/blog/solar-radiation-spectrum/

      Green is only bright to us because of our cone sensitivities overlapping.

      • bradrn 6 hours ago

        > It actually peaks between magenta and blue

        No, it actually peaks wherever you want it to peak, depending on how you plot it: https://www.oceanopticsbook.info/view/light-and-radiometry/l...

        • thaumasiotes 4 hours ago

          From the link, what appears to be the crux of the issue:

          > Figure 1 shows plots of [solar] energy irradiance as functions of both wavelength and frequency. The red line is the solar irradiance at the top of the atmosphere. The green curve in the left panel is the corresponding blackbody irradiance for a temperature of 5782 K, reduced by the distance of the Earth from the Sun

          > The peak of the blackbody irradiance spectrum is at 501 nm for a temperature of 5782 K. This corresponds to a frequency of ν = c/λ = 5.98 ⋅ 10¹⁴ Hz.

          > The right panel of the plot shows the same solar data plotted as a function of frequency, along with the corresponding blackbody spectrum

          > Note that when plotted as a function of frequency, the solar and blackbody spectra have their maxima near 3.40 ⋅ 10¹⁴ Hz, which is not the frequency corresponding to the maximum when plotted as a function of wavelength in the left panel. Indeed, 3.40 ⋅ 10¹⁴ Hz corresponds to a wavelength of λ = c/ν = 880 nm.

          > In other words, when plotted as a function of wavelength, the solar irradiance is a maximum near 500 nm, in the visible, whereas the maximum is at 880 nm, in the near infrared, when the spectrum is plotted as a function of frequency.

          [emphasis original]

          > This occurs because the relationship between wavelength and frequency is not linear, so that a unit wavelength interval corresponds to a different size of frequency interval for each wavelength: ∣dν∣ = ∣c / λ² dλ∣.

          However, it continues:

          > Figure 2 shows the solar photon irradiance [number of photons per second, as opposed to number of joules delivered per second] and the corresponding blackbody spectra

          > Now the maximum is at 635 nm when plotted as a function of wavelength and at 1563 nm, in the short-wave infrared, when plotted as a function of frequency. The maxima are at still different locations if the spectra are plotted as a function of wavenumber.

          This time the emphasis is mine. This contradicts the explanation given above, that the curve peaks in different places along different scales because those scales are nonlinearly related. The relationship between wavenumber and frequency is linear. Why does that lead to a different plotted peak?

          Are we measuring wavenumber somewhere within the atmosphere (where?), rather than in a vacuum? That would mean that different frequencies of light had different velocities, complicating the relationship between frequency and wavenumber. But it would also be a strangely artifactual way to represent solar irradiance. What's so special about wherever it is that we standardized wavenumber measurements?

          • bubblyworld 4 hours ago

            Irradiance is a density function - it's telling you how much energy or how many photons are hitting a given area per unit of the dependent/spectral variable (you can see this by inspecting the units in the plots, for instance).

            This means that changes of variable come with an additional factor - the Jacobian. You cannot simply substitute the relationship directly into the formula for the one spectral variable, like you would for a "point function" (you need to account for the change of unit in the y-axis too, if you like, not just the x-axis).

            It's not explained well but this is fundamentally what causes the moving peaks.

            • adornKey 36 minutes ago

              This subject is also a wonderful example why the maximum of a densitiy function has very little meaning. The amount of energy is an integral over the densitiy function, and if you look at the solar-spectrum, you'll notice, that the bulk of the energy is in the infrared.

            • thaumasiotes 36 minutes ago

              > Irradiance is a density function - it's telling you how much energy or how many photons are hitting a given area per unit of the [in]dependent/spectral variable (you can see this by inspecting the units in the plots, for instance).

              Well, I can see that in the labels on the y-axis, but I assumed it was a mistake.

              So you have a graph that tells you that, for light of wavelength 1000 nm, measured irradiance is 3.5e+18 photons per square meter per second per nanometer.

              And since there "are" 1000 nanometers (?!?), this means that the actual irradiance is 3.5e+21 photons per square meter per second.

              Or does the "per nanometer" really mean something less stupid than that? What's being measured? What kind of nanometers are those on the y-axis?

              • bubblyworld 9 minutes ago

                Not quite - the nanometres on the y-axis are "deltas" of your spectral variable. The density allows you to answer questions like "how much power am I getting per square metre in a range [A, B] of wavelengths". You would integrate your density between A and B to obtain the value.

                For example, pick some small epsilon "k" close to 0 in units of nm. Then in your example, the amount of irradiation contributed by the small window [1000-k, 1000+k] nm of wavelengths is roughly equal to k*3.5e18 photons per square metre per second (you can check that the units work out). The smaller k is, the more accurate the approximation. If you want to get an answer for a larger interval you can break it up into lots of k-sized pieces and sum the results up. Recall from calculus that this is exactly what integration is (yes, I know the truth is a little more complicated in general measure theory).

                Does that help? It's a bit like a continuous probability distribution, in the sense that to get an actual probability out of it you have to integrate. Formally a mathematician would say that a density corresponds to a "measure" over the space of all possible values of your spectral values.

      • __MatrixMan__ 5 hours ago

        I may have been a bit lazy there and imagined the distribution as Gaussian despite having seen charts that indicate otherwise. I'm glad you pointed that out.

        But the question remains... Why do plants reflect light so well at the frequency where my cone sensitivities overlap? Mere coincidence would be believable, but it seems to also hint at something about the relationship between myself and those plants.

        • adrian_b an hour ago

          You have a kind of cone with maximum sensitivity in green, because that is what plants reflect, not the other way around.

          You have another kind of cone with maximum sensitivity in blue, because that is what the clear sky provides.

          So a mammal looking upwards (or forward for a tree-dwelling animal) can distinguish easily leaves from sky.

          Monkeys and us have another kind of cone with maximum sensitivity in red for 2 reasons, blood is red and many mature fruits have reddish colors (including orange and purple).

          The redness of blood matters not only for noticing wounds, but also for detecting emotions on the faces of mates or adversaries (due to increased blood flow; for some monkeys the emotions may be seen not only on faces, but also on other hairless areas, e.g. buttocks).

        • LeratoAustini 3 hours ago

          We often hear about the relationship between perception of red and berries (food). Perhaps for survival in very dry areas: green == plants == water?

      • scythe 8 hours ago

        That graph shows sunlight in the upper atmosphere, but at sea level, the 400-450 nanometer blues are partially scattered out. The peak we see on the ground is broader, and centered more around 450-550 nanometers, a range that tends more towards teal or "Miami green". Wikipedia shows both spectra:

        https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sunlight#/media/File:Solar_spe...

        I think this is also why the sky appears to be a deeper, darker blue at higher altitudes.

        • cornstalks 7 hours ago

          The chart I linked shows both upper atmosphere and sea level irradiance, for what it's worth. Though it's possible the chart simply isn't the most accurate. I only did casual research.

          Doing more research, looking at [1] (the data source for that Wikipedia image), it looks like the peak is between 480 nm (cyan) and 540 nm (lime green) for "global tilt" and 480 nm to 580 nm (yellow) for "direct + circumsolar" (I have no idea what the difference is between "global tilt" and "direct + circumsolar").

          Interesting. It seems like the chart in my original comment isn't as precise as I believed.

          [1]: https://www.nrel.gov/grid/solar-resource/spectra-am1.5

    • o11c 11 hours ago

      It could also be to prevent overstimulation; "maximize energy" is not really the goal. A lot of plants can die from too much Sun unless their other inputs are just right (plenty of water, etc.).

    • gettingoverit 9 hours ago

      > something to do with the physics of absorbing light to drive a chemical reaction

      Exactly that. Blue does two steps of the process, while red does only one. There's a cost for synthesizing all that machinery, so absorbing green would just be not worth it.

      > 450nm and 680nm are not harmonics

      In fact they're in 3:2 ratio with 1% margin. But they don't have to be. Take a look at fluorescence: it converts one wavelength to another, and they don't have to be multiples of each other. Once photon gets absorbed onto a chemical, the electronic structure of the molecule decides what will happen to it.

    • privatelypublic 11 hours ago

      Have you looked into band-gaps?

      Also remember that these are random processes with selection pressure keeping those who survive to reproduce. Assigning a will to such processes makes them and the results harder to understand- imho.

      Theres probably something more efficient at converting light into simple sugars.

    • pinkmuffinere 12 hours ago

      This is fascinating, I’d never realized there is this seeming-paradox! Thanks for mentioning it

    • cheschire 11 hours ago

      Maybe it was in response to an extinction level event that filtered sunlight for a long time, removing green but allowing primarily only blue or red.

      • selcuka 11 hours ago

        It still doesn't explain the need to reflect green, though. They could have evolved to be black and absorb all energy.

        • AngryData 2 hours ago

          Well I think the question then becomes can they use that much energy without cooking themselves alive? Maybe the entire spectrum is simply too much incoming energy to dissipate effectively and blue and red absorption are either more efficient or is easier to accomplish than green.

    • NoImmatureAdHom 11 hours ago

      Here's a recent take: https://www.quantamagazine.org/why-are-plants-green-to-reduc...

      TLDR: Plants are running an energy-harvesting system that can only respond so quickly to changes in light input. Making use of green would cause variance to be large enough that the gains would not offset the losses. So, avoid green and have lower variance --> higher energy capture on average.

      • amelius 2 hours ago

        > Plants are running an energy-harvesting system that can only respond so quickly to changes in light input.

        That would be easy to test, I suppose.

        In fact, perhaps we're already doing so by letting plants live in our offices with 60Hz flicker, and perhaps higher frequency flicker caused by LEDs and PWMs.

        In short, I'm not buying this theory just yet.

    • amelius 12 hours ago

      Red is nature's warning signal, and blue was already taken by the sky, so the only option left was green.

      Just kidding of course, it is an interesting question.

      • amelius 4 hours ago

        Maybe it has something to do with keeping the albedo of the planet at a higher value.

  • andyfilms1 13 hours ago

    This is a good biological explanation. The physical explanation is, if the sensitivities didn't overlap, our spectral sensitivity would not be continuous. There would be valleys of zero sensitivity between the cones, and a continuous wavelength sweep would result in us seeing black bands between colors.

    • aetherson 12 hours ago

      Gray bands, or more realistically just desaturated bands. There'd still be sensitivity to light through rods (black and white), and even if the peaks of wavelength sensitivity were highly separated there would still be some cone response to wavelengths that didn't stimulate them strongly.

  • im3w1l 13 hours ago

    > So, only some (heterozygous) females in these species typically have full trichromatic vision

    Wow that's wild how heterozygousity can be that helpful. Makes you wonder if there are other genes like that.

TheAceOfHearts 16 hours ago

To me it looked like the circle outline had a shimmering aura, it felt very magical. This was a incredibly delightful experience so I just want to say thanks for posting it.

When the circle was around the halfway point of shrinking the color looked the most vivid for me, so be sure to wait the whole duration.

  • Cthulhu_ 7 minutes ago

    The one with a red circle on a brown background had a really interesting effect; before the circle starts to shrink, the background becomes the same colour as the circle, then moving your eyes made a bright red or green circle appear, each on their opposite side.

  • isoprophlex 5 hours ago

    At the end, the green circle had a very intense grainy, hypersaturated quality for me. Not like the shimmering aura that shrinks with the red circle, but still there was something magical going on.

    I looked away, and then back to the screen, and the effect was gone. It was only in my head. Wow.

    A super fun, delightful experience indeed.

    • renewiltord 5 hours ago

      I, too, had this view. It was flecked with blue. Like a linen sheet it had those lines.

  • uncircle 3 hours ago

    The aura was a very brilliant green, which reminded me of an hallucinogenic experience I had. I was sat with friends in the city centre, my brain altered by chemical substances, and I spotted a guy 100m away with the most brilliant and beautiful green shoes. It was an amazing sight, standing out from everything else.

    Sober, I later realised that they were normal green shoes, but in my no-brain-filter state, I was able to appreciate that we have many more green cones in our retina than red or blue, and normally the mix dial for green in our brain is kept pretty low not to overpower the other colours. The animation once again raised this dial to show how powerful is our raw perception of that colour.

    (The evolutionary reason is that we spent a lot of time in vegetation or on trees, and it's very useful to be able to distinguish things and perceive small movements in a sea of green.)

  • AlphaAndOmega0 7 hours ago

    To me, it seemed to be a visual equivalent of that auditory trick where a note seems to descend or ascend in pitch indefinitely. The outer aura of color seemed to be shrinking constantly.

  • soared 13 hours ago

    You can also look at the background about halfway through and get a large circle of the new color, the same size as the original circle.

  • bozhark 14 hours ago

    Similar, an extremely bright and magnificent teal-ish green with a vibrant yellow edge was dancing around the edge of the circle

peterisza 14 hours ago

A bit unrelated but I found this interesting: water is transparent only within a very narrow band of the electromagnetic spectrum, so living organisms evolved sensitivity to that band, and that's what we now call "visible light".

http://hyperphysics.phy-astr.gsu.edu/hbase/Chemical/imgche/w...

  • Terr_ 8 hours ago

    I like to joke that while nitrogen gas is the most common thing around us, we are blind to it. Of course, that's a feature, since it allows us to perceive everything else further away, instead of stumbling through a perpetual fog.

    This location-dependent tradeoff is something to think about when it comes to "false color" images in astronomy. If some aliens described Earth as "a boring uniform nitrogen-colored ball", we'd probably be a little offended at their ophthalmo-centrism, and tell them that the fault lies in their eyes, not in our planet.

    • the21st 3 hours ago

      Is there a camera that would show me what our world looks like through the eyes of these hypothetical aliens? Would love to see it.

      • Terr_ 2 hours ago

        It would probably be some application of spectral imaging [0], highly dependent on what data you choose to capture based on your assumptions about how the aliens see.

        Even if you have a mathematical "photo of a planet of nitrogen gas clouds", that leaves the problem of how to present it to humans, since we have no concept of what "nitrogen gas color" is supposed to be.

        https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hyperspectral_imaging

  • cmrx64 4 hours ago

    visible light is also the last octave before you hit ionizing radiation. it’s very energetic. good for harnessing in chemical processes. not so energetic that the electrons leave the party.

  • andyferris 12 hours ago

    Given the fluid inside your eyeball is mostly water, this is probably very related.

    It’s interesting (kinda optimal) that different cones explore near both edges.

  • dennis_jeeves2 13 hours ago

    Interesting, given that most life is water based, most life will respond the most to this spectrum.

  • doubleunplussed 9 hours ago

    I thought it was mostly that those are the wavelengths output out by the sun.

    But I guess it could be both.

    • davrosthedalek 8 hours ago

      The sun is very close to a black body radiator, so all wavelength. The atmosphere and water filters a lot.

      It is actually quite strange that plants are green -- that's the wavelength the atmosphere lets through particularly well, so would be particular good to be absorbed instead of reflected, for energy production. It seems nature hasn't come up with a good, cheap way to move the absorption into that wavelength.

      • doubleunplussed an hour ago

        Well, black-body radiation is still peaked around a certain range of wavelengths depending on the temperature, it's not just equal power at all wavelengths.

        Light visible to humans is at the peakiest bit of the sun's black body spectrum, see here image here: https://i.sstatic.net/kRUju.png

        Green isn't just the wavelength the atmosphere lets through the best, or the wavelength humans are most sensitive to, it's also the peak of the sun's black body spectrum.

perching_aix 13 hours ago

> If you refused to look at the animation, it’s just a bluish-green background with a red circle on top that slowly shrinks down to nothing. That’s all. But as it shrinks, you should hallucinate a very intense blue-green color around the rim.

I do not believe I have any kind or amount of colorblindness, so imagine my surprise when extremely confused I pulled the image into MS Paint, used the Color Picker tool, and found that indeed, the background has quite a bit of blue in it.

Anyhow, I cannot reproduce the illusion cited. For me the circle just blurs out and I start seeing orange.

  • stouset 13 hours ago

    I did see the illusion but I just did a double-take. That image looks just straight green to me. I suppose I could imagine it being greener somehow, but blue!?

    I have a slight deuteranomaly. I did see the illusion. Pretty!

  • ricardobeat 38 minutes ago

    I believe they are simply describing the color components, just like you don’t “see red” in a soft orange color.

    Did you wait for the black bar to finish, and the circle to start shrinking? Takes a very long time. The effect happens at the edges and disappears if you remove your focus from the center dot.

    I have mild achromatopsia and can see the effect in all color variants I tried.

  • cortesoft 8 hours ago

    Yeah, I don’t think I have any color blindness but that looked super green to me. I think I am fine at distinguishing two colors, but i am not the best at realizing the component colors I am seeing.

  • gitremote 6 hours ago

    It sounds like you have red-green color blindness. What result do you get when you take this test?

    https://www.colorblindnesstest.org/cambridge-color-test/

    • perching_aix 5 hours ago

      100% accuracy, 25/25 correct. That said, some of them were extremely difficult, way more than others. Not sure if that's intentional, so I might still have color vision deficiency per se.

      I also did the Farnsworth-Munsell 100 hue discrimination test, got a score of 28 on that - ideal being 0, and above 4 meaning something is amiss. So I don't really know what to make of this lol

      • perching_aix 3 hours ago

        Redid these tests together with a colleague. Either they are fake or they are badly coded. We double checked the ordering with a color picker and we both ordered by hue angle just fine, yet various tests would keep reporting poor results. Eventually, tests would then flip and start reporting really good scores. Got 2s and a 0 at some point.

        Now either we both fluked it the first few times or these tests are genuinely not trustworthy. I find that pretty unlikely, but what I do think I know is that I do not have CVD after all.

  • blincoln 13 hours ago

    If you make the outer colour yellow using the custom colour option, and the inner circle red, do you see a an aurora-green halo? Or if you make the outer circle yellow and the inner circle green, do you see a red halo?

    • perching_aix 13 hours ago

      > If you make the outer colour yellow using the custom colour option, and the inner circle red, do you see a an aurora-green halo?

      You mean this, right? https://dynomight.net/img/colors/generate.html?inside=ff0000...

      The background turns green (???) eventually, kind of like as if ink started to spread across it.

      Or you meant full yellow (255r, 255g, 0b) and full red (255r, 0g, 0b)?

      > Or if you make the outer circle yellow and the inner circle green, do you see a red halo?

      I used the controls this time and made the background full yellow (255r, 255g, 0b) and the inner circle full green (0r, 255g, 0b). Also adjusted the countdown speed, I realized I wasn't patient enough to wait out the 60s before ever (but that also it didn't need to be so long).

      During countdown the entire image turned green. Whenever my eyes would move a bit, I'd see either a 3D shadow depth effect or a yellow aura around the circle. When the circle started getting smaller I just saw the yellow aura. Whenever I'd drastically move my eyes, the entire background would revert to yellow, but would quickly go back to seeing green.

      I don't really see them being unusually saturated though, but maybe I just don't have a good grasp on what to expect. Maxed out R/G/B or C/M/Y all strike me as super saturated from the get-go.

      • gitremote 5 hours ago

        Do you have a setting on that shifts the screen colors towards red in the evening to avoid blue light?

        For the first question, I see a green halo.

        For the second question, I see something in between what you and blincoln see. The halo I see is definitely orange, not red. During countdown, the outside of the circle became greenish-yellow, but I could distinguish between the circle and outside the circle.

pb060 5 hours ago

Incidentally, it’s also a demonstration that you shouldn’t use high contrast in typography. When you start the test you can clearly see the lines of text retained on your retina.

  • rvnx 5 hours ago

    The same with MacBooks in dark mode, once you turn around you can see large horizontal lines separated at regular intervals that are maybe due to the refresh rate of the screen (or something else, if someone knows)

    • chromakode 4 hours ago

      In my experience these are after exposures from lines of text. They get blurred together into indistinct lines because your eye focus moves between words, superimposing them.

  • ErigmolCt 4 hours ago

    Sometimes "crisp" quickly turns into "burned into your vision"

blincoln 13 hours ago

I did a custom combination of yellow outer field, blue inner circle, and got a vibrant purple halo, which is not what I expected. I assumed it would be "yellow++", based on what I know about the human eye's colour sensitivity.

I didn't expect a strong effect, because the overlap between blue and red/green is so much less than the overlap between red and green, but bright purple is close to the opposite of what I expected. I'm genuinely puzzled.

  • dreijs 10 hours ago

    That is interesting - I tried the setup you suggested and actually did see something that could be described as yellow++, with maybe a hint of gold/orange

    It was also very intense, like staring into the sun. I observed this for both of the two default yellow tints that I could choose via the (Windows?) color picker

    I have no idea why this would differ between individuals

    Edit: link below with FFFF00 for yellow

    https://dynomight.net/img/colors/generate.html?inside=0000ff...

  • riknos314 8 hours ago

    Can you share settings and/or a link so an exported SVG?

anigbrowl 10 hours ago

Painters have been aware of this distinction for years. I encourage interested readers to get a good artist's book on color or just head for your local art store and explore the differences between pthalo and viridian greens (or any of many other surprisingly different tonal clashes).

  • ErigmolCt 4 hours ago

    There's a reason artists obsess over their palettes

weinzierl 14 hours ago

"If you’re colorblind, I don’t think these will work, though I’m not sure."

Should work for anomalous trichromats (by far the majority of people with color deficiencies) but probably with less intensity.

"Folks with deuteranomaly have M cones, but they’re shifted to respond more like L cones."

I don't think this is true. What would the difference between deutan and protan then be?

"Why do you hallucinate that crazy color? I think the red circle saturates the hell out of your red-sensitive L cones. Ordinarily, the green frequencies in the background would stimulate both your green-sensitive M cones and your red-sensitive L cones, due to their overlapping spectra. But the red circle has desensitized your red cones, so you get to experience your M cones firing without your L cones firing as much, and voilà—insane color."

I think only people with missing L cone (Protanopia) or M cone (Deiteranopia) would not experience the phenomenon at all.

Maybe this could be used as a new type of color deficiency test?

kadoban 16 hours ago

I am curious how these work for people with common kinds of colorblindness. The author mentions at the end that they likely don't work for that case, but they don't seem to have spent much time thinking about it.

Would it be possible to generate ones that _would_ work for specific kinds of colorblindness? Or is the entire concept doomed due to the specific way(s) that colorblind eyes are messed up?

  • tgv an hour ago

    Didn't work for me. I notice that the color around the circle changes, but it's just a palish blue-something, not a new color, certainly not intense. Too much overlap, I guess. Perhaps it works when changing the background to blue?

    I fail on the first page of the Ishihara (?) test. When using those colorblindness filters in photoshop/-like apps, the protanopia and deuteranopia filters do very little for me. No 'real' diagnosis. I can see red and green though, but the shape needs to be big, and the color should be quite saturated. Reading resistors is really hard.

  • dentemple 15 hours ago

    I have deuteranomaly, and the hallucination worked for me, and it did appear like a crazy saturated blue-green ring around the shrinking red circle.

    I suspect, however, that those of us with deuteranomaly probably see a different blue-green than normal-sighted folks due to the bent color cones.

    The real question is, what about the folks with Deuteranopia (no working green cones at all)?

    Deuteranomaly, though, is still probably the best place to start since that's the big one that affects (some say) up to 10% of all males. Every other form of colorblindness affects a much slimmer percentage of the population.

  • qayxc 16 hours ago

    The red inside, reddish-orange outside was a little strange - I'm not colour blind, but have a really hard time distinguishing shades. As soon as I focused on the white dot, the red circle started to blend with the background and disappeared completely (was just one single colour for me). Only when it started shrinking did I hallucinated a faint green aura around it until it was gone.

  • kookamamie 16 hours ago

    The animation worked for me, I'm red-green colorblind.

  • tricolon 13 hours ago

    I have red-green weakness but only saw a lighter green around the circle as it became smaller.

ErigmolCt 4 hours ago

The part about bypassing our cone overlap with direct stimulation is wild, like hacking your brain's display settings

pb060 5 hours ago

I don’t understand why you need the animation. If you point at the green background after staring the red one for a while, you can see a whole circle of the saturated color.

  • spragl 2 hours ago

    I agree, that you dont. As far as I can tell, the effect comes from superimposing an after-image on some other image.

osamagirl69 16 hours ago

It is incredible to see a concept going from 'optical table of sensitive equipment fraught with numerous safety concerns' to 'here is a 1 kB svg animation, stare at it for 1 minute' in 3 months.

Enjoy your forbidden color, you earned it!

  • layer8 15 hours ago

    The article however concludes: “So do the illusions actually take you outside the natural human color gamut? Unfortunately, I’m not sure. I can’t find much quantitative information about how much your cones are saturated when you stare at red circles. My best guess is no, or perhaps just a little.”

armchairhacker 16 hours ago

This is really cool. Tangentially, it's an example of an important life lesson, "work smarter not harder". To see the impossible color, you could build a super-expensive, super-complicated laser to directly stimulate the exact cells; or you could desensitize the other ones with an optical illusion that works on a personal device (effectively zero cost and minimal complexity since it uses existing technology).

Not to say the laser is a waste, despite the above I'd argue it's very useful. It lets us test how effectively the above actually works, and has other applications.

  • do_not_redeem 15 hours ago

    This optical illusion isn't some brand new thing. It's been widely known since I was a child, and surely hundreds of years before that.

    The laser system results in a stronger perceptual effect than you get from the illusion alone. We didn't have the technology to build it until recently. I'm certain the people who built it knew about the illusion, and it's probably what inspired the experiment in the first place.

  • fortyseven 14 hours ago

    That is a notion that is far easier to make in hindsight.

ComplexSystems 15 hours ago

Is this just my device, or is there no way to use this roll-your-own SVG generator to actually roll your own? I can only pick from a tiny subset of preset colors, most of which seem super random and desaturated and not what I want. There's no FFFF00 yellow, for instance. Is there some way to enter an arbitrary RGB color that I am not seeing? If not, why on Earth write such an interesting article, advertise this custom SVG generator and then build the interface that way? :/

  • altairprime 15 hours ago

    Does tapping on the horizontal color box to the right of the sentence “Select any color” under heading “Inside Color (Circle)” open a color picker? If not, perhaps your browser has a defective <input type=color> implementation, i.e. Firefox for Android [1796343]?

_Microft 16 hours ago

Open the experiment animation and refresh the page multiple times to refresh the countdown while looking at the white pixel (from the same point of view) to get an even more impressive effect.

sampl3username 16 hours ago

Using psychedelics, specifically 2C-B and LSD, you can also see very saturated colors you don't normally see in daily life. I see very saturated magentas.

  • louthy 15 hours ago

    I wonder how much of this is ‘seeing’ and how much is emergent in the brain due to the drug. I suspect the latter, but that’s just opinion.

    • dr_dshiv 15 hours ago

      “how much of this is ‘seeing’ and how much is emergent in the brain”

      Yeah… it’s gonna be hard to distinguish those in the best of circumstances.

    • sampl3username 14 hours ago

      Seeing is also an illusion by the brain, all colors are in your head.

      • louthy 14 hours ago

        I realise that, I’m just speculating on when the effect emerges. Whether it’s because of changes in the cones (which are the tips of nuerons) or a later emergent property.

        • GuB-42 13 hours ago

          Most likely an emergent property. Psychedelics affect serotonin pathways, I don't think that cone cells and the first layers of neurons behind them have serotonin receptors.

  • neom 15 hours ago

    Everything is quite intense that way with 2C-B, very very rich, but in a more psilocybin way than LSD. 2C-B is super weird, I find it hard to pinpoint what it is about 2C-B that is so unique among the phenethylamines/tryptamines.

tanepiper 15 hours ago

Interesting colours coming out of it - a while back I suspected I have https://en.m.wikipedia.org/wiki/Tetrachromacy since I was able to describe colours more vividly that others, and certain plants for me like Verbena have a glow around them.

  • dr_dshiv 14 hours ago

    Some people with two X chromosomes have this ability. And all birds.

    If you are bored, try to get Gemini/claude to make a color wheel for birds or tetrachromats.

    An aside: Recently I learned that birds are reptiles. That hurt my brain and I’m still recovering. Especially since the modern dinosaur exhibit claiming this fact contradicted the 1980s era reptiles exhibit down the hall (both at the British museum).

    • nocoiner 12 hours ago

      What are birds? We just don’t know.

      • GuB-42 10 hours ago

        Dinosaurs are birds. Birds are reptiles.

        Also, reptiles are technically fishes, and so are we, but we are not reptiles.

    • im3w1l 12 hours ago

      I think I would make a color triangle, except I would remove blue from it, and instead have the corners be three different colors in the red-green part of the spectrum. I guess 530, 545, and 560nm.

  • Aardwolf 15 hours ago

    I'd love it if there was someone with tetrachromacy who also knows a bit about color theory and perception and can talk about it!

    How do you describe the experience scientifically? Do you get a whole bunch of extra colors you'd want to give a distinct name since they're so clearly different from the standard trichromatic colors?

    Is a computer screen annoying because it can only produce a subset of the colors you can see?

    Do you notice that you have a fourth parameter or dimension in the colors you see, so would want a 4th component in RGB, HSV, etc... color sliders? E.g. for our HSL, would the fourth parameter be hue-like, saturation-like, lightness like or some completely novel other thing? If hue like, do the hues also form a 2D sphere or torus like topology similar to how our trichromatic hue forms a circle?

    I'd expect at least twice or 3x as many named colors, since for every regular color (red, green, blue, yellow, orange, purple, pink, grey, brown, black, white, ...) , you'd have a fourth dimension altering it that can be low, medium or high in value ...

    E.g. for our yellow, you'd have yellow with not the extra signal, a bit of the extra signal or lots of the extra signal. Is this the case or not? Perhaps the overlapping reduces it, but as said in the article trichromats also have overlap yet we definitely see a lot more distinct colors than dichromats.

vintermann 15 hours ago

Telenor's net nanny (which I didn't ask for) has decided that dynomight.net is dangerous and DNS blocked it.

  • perching_aix 13 hours ago

    So change your router settings to offer a different default DNS via DHCP? Maybe configure your device(s) to use some other specific DNS servers rather than the ones offered via DHCP?

    For the site operator: the domain is present in the Spamhaus DBL (Domain Block List), which is presumably why these lovely gents are having issues, might wanna check that out.

  • DavidVoid 14 hours ago

    Ran into the same thing. Changing to a non-default DNS fixed it.

rezmason 14 hours ago

I just want a display with a primary at 460nm. That's all I ask.

gcr 16 hours ago

What is the animation supposed to be like? I see just a black bar on the left narrowing, but nothing else happens. The red circle and green background and white dot didn’t change. (iOS 26 beta, iPhone 15)

  • gblargg 5 hours ago

    The bar is counting down time to wear out the receptors in your eye (you need to be staring at the center the whole time). THEN the illusion begins.

  • gpderetta 16 hours ago

    After the black bar disappears, the circle start shrinking and on the boundary you can indeed an intense azure/green colour.for me quickly flickering the eyes left and right did temporarily increase the patches of intense colour.

  • solardev 15 hours ago

    It's not the animation itself that does anything magical, but the afterimage (I think that's what you would call it?) your eye produces after you stare at the dot for long enough. The black bar is just a countdown timer for the impatient.

    But try to maintain laser focus on the central dot, not letting your eyes move or blink if you can help it. Once the black bar depletes, the circle should start shrinking, and around its periphery (like an eclipse) should be some incredibly vivid, super saturated colors.

  • satellite2 16 hours ago

    After the black bar finish narrowing the red circle gets smaller slowly

fabiospampinato 16 hours ago

Now we need to know from the people that experienced the laser how different this hallucination feels compared to that. Very cool stuff!

lubujackson 15 hours ago

Cross your eyes like a magic eye image when it's shrinking and the vivid color will expand to a larger patch.

juliushuijnk 15 hours ago

To get 'speed up' the effect, move your face close (to the red) then away from your screen.

deadbabe 9 hours ago

The green I see around the red circle is exactly the color of a green traffic light bulb when lit, which has a hint of blue to it and is not actually pure green.

nvch 15 hours ago

It's enough to stare at anything for a few minutes without moving eyes to get similar effects and hallucinations.

We see with good resolution only a small part of our visual field. Perhaps the brain starts to "invent" what's there it we don't give it information by constantly moving eyes.

As a more advanced version, they say that fire kasina practice may produce very interesting visual effects.